US psychologists to review gay ‘conversion therapy’

Time magazine reports that the American Psychological Association is conducting a review of their policy on treating gay clients, with a particular focus on reassessing guidelines around controversial therapies aimed at assisting gay clients to ‘go straight’.

The APA already have guidelines for working with gay, lesbian and bisexual clients that make it clear that homosexuality is not to be considered a mental illness.

However, a minority of therapists, usually linked to conservative Christian organisations, practice a type of therapy known as ‘reparative’ or ‘conversion therapy’ to assist gay clients who wish to be straight.

This practice is strongly opposed by gay rights groups, who suggest that it is at best futile and at worst harmful, and feel that it is the distress of discrimination that pushes people into starting such therapies.

Neither the current guidelines, nor the APA policy statement on lesbian and gay issues, makes any mention of this issue – something that the review panel is likely to consider in detail.

Gay rights advocates are hoping that the APA will denounce ‘reparative’ or ‘conversion’ therapies while conservative religious groups have written to the APA to defend their work with consenting clients.

As with many of these policy reviews, the arguments centre as much on who has been selected for the review panel as the debate itself:

Conservatives contend that the review’s outcome is preordained because the task force is dominated by gay-rights supporters. “We’re concerned,” said Carrie Gordon Earll of Focus on the Family. “The APA does not have a good track record of listening to other views.”

Joseph Nicolosi, a leading proponent of reparative therapy, predicted the task force would propose a ban of the practice — and he vowed to resist such a move. Nicolosi, who was rejected as a task force nominee, is president of the National Association for Research and Therapy of Homosexuality.

Clinton Anderson, director of the APA’s Lesbian, Gay and Bisexual Concerns Office, insisted the panel would base its findings on scientific research, not ideology. He defended the decision to reject certain conservative applicants to the task force. “We cannot take into account what are fundamentally negative religious perceptions of homosexuality ‚Äî they don’t fit into our world view,” Anderson said.

Link to Time article ‘Psychologists to Review Stance on Gays’.

The relative exposure of our respective arses

In 1980, the New York Review of Books published a heated exchange between psychologist and IQ researcher Hans Eysenck and biologist and IQ skeptic Stephen Jay Gould.

It remains a classic moment in the IQ debate, not least because of the entertaining mud slinging.

The exchange followed a review of Arthur Jensen’s book Bias in Mental Testing by Stephen Jay Gould in which he slams the concept of IQ and general intelligence – the idea that there is a core resource of psychological ability that most mental tasks draw upon.

In the first exchange, Hans Eysenck, one of the world’s most famous psychologists at the time, wrote to the magazine throwing doubt on pretty much everything Gould had to say.

Gould makes a valiant comeback, dismissing most the arguments as attacking him rather than his claims, but notes that one is “the only meat in a sandwich surrounded by too much very stale (if not moldy) bread”.

You can always tell a scientific argument has got interesting when it wanders off the original point and focuses on who can see whose arse.

Eysenck starts the second exchange with “It is always interesting to note the reactions of a critic who is caught with his pants down, and Stephen Jay Gould’s reply to my letter is no exception”.

Gould, unable to resist carrying the metaphor, replies: “I don’t wish to engage Mr. Eysenck in a protracted debate about the relative exposure of our respective arses; nonetheless, I can’t resist noting that his initial remark surprised me because I thought I had caught him in the same unenviable posture he ascribes to me.”

The debate was heated largely because of Eysenck’s controversial views on intelligence. He suggested that IQ was largely determined by genetics and that small but significant differences could be seen between races as a result.

He was accused of being racist, but he claimed he was simply reporting the data from his studies and noted, in his defence, that he found that Asian people typically came out with the highest IQ – hardly the views of a white supremacist.

Gould was shortly to publish The Mismeasure of Man, a book pouring scorn on the whole concept of IQ and arguing that the tests had serious cultural biases built into them so they were never going to be a fair comparison.

The exchange is worth reading in full both to get a flavour of the debate (essentially the same points are still being made today) and, of course, just for sheer entertainment value.

Link to original book review.
Link to round one.
Link to round two.

Edelman on neural darwinism and consciousness

Biologist Gerald Edelman is interviewed in Discover magazine about his views on the brain’s own internal ‘natural selection’ process and its possible role in the development of consciousness.

Edelman won the Nobel Prize in 1972 for his work on antibodies, but later turned to neuroscience and is keen to crack the problem of consciousness.

He argues that pathways in the brain are created by a process akin to ‘natural selection, where the most useful survive.

In the first few months of life, the neurons, on average, are more connected with each other than later in life.

If you click here you can see a graph of the number of synapses (inter-neuron connections) present in the human visual cortex by age.

According to the study that this graph is taken from, the peak time for synaptic connections is 6 months old. After that the number rapidly decreases.

This happens because connections that aren’t used disappear on a ‘use it or lose it’ basis, and the ones that are left form the more permanent connections in the brain.

In other words, from all the random variation, the weak connections die out and the strongest survive.

Edelman also argues that this principal applies to larger patterns of activity in the brain – with past and ongoing experience determining what can be considered useful.

Edelman talks about his theory and how he thinks it is crucial in understanding consciousness, and also how his research group is attempting to build robots based on the same principal.

Link to Edelman interview in Discover magazine.

John Nash speaks to American psychiatry

John Nash, Nobel prize winning mathematician and subject of the Oscar winning biopic A Beautiful Mind, delivered a speech to the American Psychiatric Association’s annual conference. In his talk, he suggested that mental illness may be the result of the otherwise healthy evolution of mental diversity.

Applying his specialized understanding of “game theory” to an analysis of mental illness and his own experience with psychosis, the 79-year-old Nobel Laureate suggested that severe mental illness exists in nature as a consequence of the diversification of species, and that it may serve the needs of adaptation by its not infrequent association with genius.

It is a line of thinking that has been followed by such renowned psychiatric researchers as Nancy Andreasen, M.D., and Kay Redfield Jamison, Ph.D.

At the same conference, model and actress Brooke Shields spoke to the conference about her own experience of post-partum depression – the depressive disorder that occurs after giving birth in about 1 out of every 10 women.

Link to Psychiatric News on Nash’s speech (via FuriousSeasons).
Link to Psychiatric News on Shield’s speech.

Neuropsychopharmacology: The Fifth Generation

The American College of Neuropsychopharmacology have made a huge text book freely available online that covers the cutting edge of pretty much everything we know about how drugs affect the mind and brain.

Psychopharmacology is the science of how drugs affect the mind. You can do this without a huge understanding of brain function. You can just see how different drugs affect people’s mental state.

This was pretty much how many of the early drug treatments in psychiatry were discovered.

For example, the first antipsychotic, chlorpromazine, was developed in the 1950s as an antiemetic, a drug to prevent vomiting.

However, the French doctor Henri Laborit noticed that it induced a sort of ‘indifference’ to the world, and wondered whether it might help calm patients with mental illness who were agitated.

It was discovered that this drug was the first effective treatment for psychosis, and for several decades, psychopharmacology research simply tested various derivatives without a good understanding of how they were affecting the brain.

Neuropsychopharmacology adds neuroscience into the mix, and attempts to explain how drugs have their effect by studying how they interact with the biology of the brain.

It’s an incredibly important science, not only for the purpose of developing new treatments, but also for understanding how any drug (be it aspirin, cocaine or caffeine) has its effect.

The online text book, entitled Neuropsychopharmacology: The Fifth Generation reviews a huge, and I mean HUGE, amount of research into this area.

It’s an academic text, so is very in-depth, but is a fantastic resource to have freely available on the net.

Link to Neuropsychopharmacology: The Fifth Generation.

Neurobiology of addiction in Time Magazine

Time magazine has an article that investigates the neuroscience of addiction and why some people find it so hard to give up drink and drugs.

The article takes an approach to addiction know as the ‘disease model’.

In its strongest form, this theory suggests that some people have a particular neurological weakness that makes them more likely to become addicted when they encounter certain substances.

Essentially, it suggests that the ‘disease’, often considered to be genetic in origin, exists before the problem behaviour.

The actual addiction is thought to be an interaction between the disease and the substance which leads to compulsive substance taking and seeking.

However, it’s also important to realise that many of the most addictive drugs directly affect a system that is also involved reward processing.

In other words, the thing that makes you feel good also affects the system that helps you judge how valuable things are.

This tends to affect your judgement of how bad you want something, and, over time, the system becomes more sensitive so it increases desire more easily,

This is known as the incentive-sensitization view and is one of the most influential neurobiological theories of addiction [pdf].

However, it has largely been developed on the basis of animal research and has been criticised for ignoring social factors.

For example, Jim Orford’s influential book Excessive Appetites (ISBN 0471499471) notes that heavy consumption of the same substance can have a very different impact depending on the cultural context.

Other than its (perhaps deliberately) narrow focus, the Time article is a competent look at the neurobiology of addiction.

UPDATE: Grabbed from the comments – a sharp-eyed observation from Resa:

I was reading this article the other day, and to my amusement, the pituitary gland had been incorrectly labeled as the amygdala in the illustration of the dopamine reward pathway. This little mishap is present in both online and print versions of the article.

You can see the offending diagram here.

Link to Time article ‘How We Get Addicted’.

Wellcome galleries of the mind and brain

Biomedical research charity the Wellcome Trust have made their image library available online. Searching for ‘brain’ or ‘psychology’ images produces hundreds of wonderful pictures from both the cutting edge of cognitive science and neurological investigations from times past.

All the images have been released under a Creative Commons licence, so you can use them without charge for non-commercial purposes.

It’s a really wonderful collection from both a scientific and artistic viewpoint and is enormously good fun to explore.

Link to front page of Wellcome Images (via BoingBoing).

Williams Syndrome and the genetics of sociability

The New York Times magazine has a great article on Williams Syndrome, the genetic disorder which leads to cognitive impairment, but with normal language and an outgoing and ‘chatty’ personality.

The article investigates the impact of this condition, but also explores what it tells us about the genetics of sociability.

Williams Syndrome results from the deletion of region q11.2 (section 11.2 of the ‘q’ or long arm) of chromosome 7. This causes 20 or so normally present genes to be missing.

The syndrome is associated with learning difficulties, slight facial differences and heart problems (not unlike several other genetic syndromes that affect the brain).

However, people with the syndrome are notable for their interest in language and conversation. They will often have a surprising vocabulary and delight in unusual words.

They also have what has been described as a ‘cocktail party personality’, meaning that they are outgoing, gregarious but often aren’t capable of dealing with deeper social issues because of their lower IQ.

The online article also has a video interview with someone with the syndrome who explains how it affects them and their family.

If you’re interested in hearing more about Williams Syndrome, NPR had an excellent radio show about it last year that’s well worth listening to.

Link to NYT article ‘The Gregarious Brain’.
Link to NPR programme ‘Williams Syndrome: It’s Not a Fairy Tale’.

SciAmMind – a slight return

Scientific American Mind usually make two of their feature articles freely available online. This issue, they seem to have substituted their articles at ‘half time’, as two new articles have become available from the June / July issue, while the two that were previously available have disappeared behind a pay wall.

One of the new articles is on how to recognise good quality advice and expertise in the cognitive, neuro and medical sciences.

The other is on the role of ‘mirror neurons’ in recovery and rehabilitation after brain injury and stroke.

I’ve got mixed feelings about this really. While its always good to have new quality mind and brain writing available, one of the attractions of SciAmMind is that freely available articles from past issues are always accessible.

Link to contents with links to freely available content (thanks Scott!)

The Cramps: Live at Napa State Mental Hospital

During a 1978 tour, psychobilly punk band The Cramps created one of the strangest moments in the history of both rock n’ roll and psychiatry when they played a gig inside Napa State Mental Hospital.

It’s hard to believe it actually happened. The story sounds more like an exaggerated rock legend than an account of a real concert, but no suspension of disbelief is needed. Someone filmed the gig.

We can only guess how the band got permission to play inside one of California’s biggest mental institutions, but play they did, to a few supporters and a fired-up crowd of psychiatric inpatients.

The footage is grainy, black and white, and chaotic, and we immediately see the band launch into a high-energy version of Mystery Plane.

The onlookers look bemused at first, a few start dancing, a few just wander.

As the first song fades, the lead singer, Lux Interior, addresses the crowd: “We’re The Cramps, and we’re from New York City and we drove 3,000 miles to play for you people.”

“Fuck you!” a patient yells back.

He cracks a smile. “And somebody told me you people are crazy! But I’m not so sure about that; you seem to be all right to me.”

The gig ascends into pure punk rock chaos.

Patients jump on stage and pogo like they were Saturday night regulars. Lux suddenly duets with a member of the crowd who grabs the mike and adds her own improvised lyrics to the mix.

One song finishes with the lead singer sprawled on the floor with two female members of the audience. One of them shouts “I got the Cramps!” Lux replies “That’s your problem, honey. I got ’em myself, and I can’t do anything with ’em, either.”

As with Johnny Cash’s landmark concert, played a decade earlier in Folsom Prison, it would be easy to assume that the onlookers are intended to be part of an ironic publicity stunt.

But one thing is striking from both of these shows: the audience wouldn’t have looked out of place at any other date on the tour.

Cash and The Cramps are unlikely bedfellows, but both took their music to the marginalised and hinted that we’re not so different from those we lock away.

OK, so The Cramps didn’t hint. Punk isn’t like that. But then again, the fans have hardly been known for their conformity either.

Link to YouTube clips of Live at Napa State Mental Hospital.

Virus linked to temporal lobe epilepsy

Open-access science journal PLoS Medicine published a recent study that suggests that infection with the herpes virus might cause temporal lobe epilepsy in some people.

The study found the virus in the brains of 11 out of 16 patients with temporal lobe epilepsy but not in those with other forms of epilepsy.

Studies that test brain tissue are often done post-mortem, on people who have died, because brain surgery is just too risky for the sake of removing samples for research.

This study is particularly impressive because it studied brain tissue from live patients.

In severe cases of epilepsy that don’t respond to medication, one option is to find which bit of the brain triggers the seizures (the ‘foci’) and surgically remove it.

This is particularly effective in people with mesial temporal lobe epilepsy, a type in which the foci is deep within the temporal lobes (mesial means ‘towards the midline’), usually stemming from disturbance in the hippocampus.

The team examined brain tissue removed in operations on 22 patients, and tested it for the presence of the human herpesvirus 6B (HHV-6B).

This type of herpes infection is incredibly common, more than 90% of the population have it. Normally, it’s completely harmless and just lies dormant in the body.

We don’t really know why, but in some people, it seems to reactivate, and is linked to neurological disorders like multiple sclerosis.

The researchers found that it was present in brain cells called astrocytes from 11 out of 16 patients with mesial temporal lobe epilepsy, but wasn’t present at all in patients with other types of epilepsy.

The image on the right is of a herpes infected astrocyte, the infection is visible due to a green marker.

They also studied one patient in more detail. He had four operations in a row, each of which reduced his seizures, until the final one left him seizure-free.

They found that the herpes virus was present most strongly in the temporal lobe tissue from the first operation, was weakly present in later operations, and wasn’t present in other brain areas.

They also found that infected brain tissue didn’t produce very much of a chemical that transports the key neurotransmitter glutamate across the brain.

If it doesn’t get transported properly, it ‘hangs around’, and because glutamate tends to make brain cells more active, too much could lead to overactivity and seizures.

To test the herpes – glutamate link, the team deliberately infected brain tissue taken from a patient without a previous infection.

In the lab, they discovered that herpes slowed the creation of the transporter chemical for glutamate, providing strong evidence for the link.

The evidence from the lab tests, the single case study, and the 22 patients, provides strong evidence that herpes infection could lead to temporal lobe epilepsy in some people.

This is an important finding because it suggests a cause for the disorder in some people, and provides a clear target which could lead to better treatments and prevention strategies.

What is still not clear is why this usually harmless infection might cause some people severe neurological problems, and remain dormant in others.

Link to PLoS Medicine paper.

2007-07-06 Spike activity

Quick links from the past week in mind and brain news:

The Times looks at the effects and treatment of childhood depression.

Sweet smells make pain more tolerable according to research covered by Mixing Memory.

The study that reported that older child had slightly higher IQ than younger children is examined and criticised by Slate.

Is synaesthesia more prevalent than previously thought? asks Cognitive Daily.

Texas prevented from executing (yet another) mentally ill prisoner by the US Supreme Court, reports the The New York Times. Most surprisingly, he was originally allowed to defend himself when obviously psychotic.

New Scientist reports that the current level of testosterone affects men’s judgments of fairness

Is there a specific electrical brain signal linked to face recognition? The Phineas Gage Fan Club investigates.

Yawning cools the brain say researchers in The New York Times.

OmniBrain finds some clay models of sensory and motor homunculi. If you don’t know what they are, have a look!

BBC News reports that contrary to popular belief, men and women speak roughly the same amount. If you follow Language Log, it’s old news of course.

Does your neighbourhood cause schizophrenia?

The results of two new studies suggest that the neighbourhood you live in affects your chance of developing schizophrenia. Surprisingly, neighbourhood seems to have no effect on your risk of bipolar disorder.

The received wisdom says that schizophrenia affects 1% of the population worldwide, with little variation between race, country and area.

The trouble is, the received wisdom is wrong. We now know that the prevalence (total amount of cases) and incidence (amount of new cases per year) varies globally, nationally and locally.

These two new studies, led by Dr James Kirkbride, investigated whether street-by-street differences also have an effect.

The research team examined every person who was treated by mental health services in South East London for a first episode of psychosis over a two-year period.

Psychosis involves delusions and hallucinations and most often leads to a diagnosis of schizophrenia or bipolar disorder, bipolar being most associated with mood problems such as soaring highs followed by crashing lows.

South East London is notable for many things, not all of them positive. It’s a high poverty, high crime, highly urbanised inner-city area.

We know that one of the single largest environmental risk factors for schizophrenia is living in an urban area, and, unsurprisingly, this corner of the UK’s captial has one of the highest rates of psychosis in the world.

The team looked at all the areas, and, controlling for the effects of age, sex and ethnicity, used statistics to test whether any differences between areas were likely to due to chance, or whether they varied enough to be confident that the critical influence was the neighbourhood.

The map on the left shows the variation between neighbourhoods. If you know South East London, click on the map to see how it relates to specific areas.

You”ll notice that the toughest, poorest areas tend to have a higher rate of schizophrenia. In comparison, the rates for bipolar disorder were largely the same wherever the team looked.

Even if you don’t know the area at all, the amount of neighbourhood variation is quite startling. In some cases, moving just a few streets could dramatically affect your mental health.

In a second study, the team looked at characteristics of the area to try and see what risk was linked to.

Some main influences stood out: poverty, ethnic fragmentation, and, surprisingly, local election voter turnout.

In fact, a 1% increase in the number of voters in local elections was linked to a 5% reduction in new cases of schizophrenia.

Voter turnout itself is hardly likely to affect mental health, but the researchers suggest it might reflect the sense of community in the neighbourhood: the more you’re concerned about your neighbourhood the more likely you are to vote on issues affecting the area.

Ethnic fragmentation is a measure of how many people of your ethnic background live in your neighbourhood.

Immigrants are known to be at greater risk of schizophrenia than other people in the country, and South East London has a large immigrant population.

This might mean that contact with a community of people who share your cultural experiences may be protective against mental illness, perhaps again suggesting that ‘community spirit’ is key for mental health.

So why do all these things affect schizophrenia and not bipolar disorder? The researchers don’t discuss it in detail probably because it’s a bit puzzling.

The truth, of course, is just outside the front door.

Link to abstract of ‘Neighbourhood variation in the incidence of psychotic disorders in Southeast London’.

Link to abstract of ‘Neighbourhood-level effects on psychoses: re-examining the role of context’.

Striking perspective shift illusion

I’ve just stumbled across a remarkably simple yet fiendishly effective visual illusion that seems to give flat images the illusions of 3D depth. It works by quickly shifting between two images of the same scene taken from slightly different perspectives.

As we’ve noted when discussing other illusions, our brain generates the experience of seeing a 3D world by making the best it can out of the two fairly poor quality flat images that fall on the back of the retina.

Visual illusions usually have their effect by taking advantage of the brain’s processes for inferring visual features.

These processes are really nothing more than educated guesses, and illusions essentially give the brain red herrings, misleading it to guess in the wrong direction, so we experience one visual feature in a context where it wouldn’t normally appear.

In this case, the illusions give the misleading impression of depth in the context of an entirely flat image.

One clue the brain uses to infer depth is occlusion – things that are near the front of a visual scene will block those at the back. They just get in the way.

In fact, the lines in cartoons or diagrams are often just a representation of the occlusion contours – the edges of where one surface hide another.

However, although we can see that a cartoon drawing of a head is a 3D representation, we don’t experience it as actually having depth. It doesn’t seem to stand out from the page.

The perspective shift illusion flips between two images taken from slightly different perspectives and this seems to add a dynamic aspect – the illusion of movement.

Movement is often crucial for determing something’s depth. Have you ever moved your head side-to-side when you see a puzzling sculpture to try and better understand its shape?

This allows us to see the relative depth of each of the occlusion contours by experiencing how much the foreground moves in relation to the background. Things nearer the front seem to ‘move quicker’ – something known as motion parallax.

I’m guessing by adding an impression of movement – some motion parallax – to a detailed photograph that already has other depth cues from the natural environment, the perspective shift illusion produces an impression of real depth.

If you want to know more about the cognitive science of 3D shape perception, there’s a great review article by available Dr James Todd available online as a pdf file.

Otherwise, just spend a few minutes checking out the impressive visual effect.

Link to perspective shift visual illusion.

The REVERB project

reverblogo.jpgA.k.a part of my day job. I’ve written a short article introducing the project. The last line summarises it pretty well (I think!)


“The goal is to reverse-engineer the computational principles that the brain uses to multi-task”, says Dr Gurney, “and to show that they work in practice, not just in theory, by incorporating them in a robot. In a sense, we’re trying to work out the real-time operating system of our own minds.”

You can read more about REVERB here and the rest of my article here